Focus on Form: Past and Present
Focus
on Form: Past and Present
(Plenary
Speech, September 23, 2016)
at
the 35th Second Language Research Forum
Teachers
College, Columbia University
By
Dr. Rod Ellis
University
of Auckland and Shanghai International Studies University
Abstract
Focus-on form (FonF) was a term introduced by Michael Long
to refer to an approach to teaching where learners’ attention is attracted to
linguistic forms as they engage in the attempt to communicate. It contrasts
with an approach that Long called “Focus-on-forms” where specific linguistic
forms are taught directly and explicitly. However, there is perhaps no
construct in SLA that has proved so malleable and shifted in meaning so much. I
will begin by considering how Long’s original definition of it has stretched
over time and then offer my own definition of the construct, arguing that the
term might best be applied to specific kinds of ‘activities’ or ‘procedures’
rather than to an ‘approach’. I will then go on to present a classification of
these different types of focus on form activities/procedures. I will also
suggest that it is important to distinguish the psycholinguistic and discoursal
dimensions of FonF. Based on this distinction, I will undertake a review of the
research that has investigated different ways in which a focus on form can be
realized and its effect on task-based performance and acquisition. In the final
part of my paper I will consider criticisms that have been leveled against the
construct. I will consider to what extent comparative studies of FonF and FonFs
can help address these criticisms and examine a number of such studies.
Finally, I will discuss what I see as the main problems of implementing FonF –
namely, the resistance it arouses in instructional contexts where for cultural
reasons FonFs has been the norm – and propose a solution.
Biography
Rod Ellis is Distinguished Professor of Applied Language
Studies in the University of Auckland, and also Cheung Kong Scholar Chair
Professor at Shanghai International Studies University. His published works
include numerous articles and books on second language acquisition and language
teaching. His most recent book is Exploring
Language Pedagogy through Second Language Acquisition Research (with
Natsuko Shintani) published by Routledge. He has also recently been elected a
Fellow of the Royal Society of New Zealand.
Long
(1988)
“…a focus on form is probably a key feature of second
language instruction…I do not think, on the other hand, that there is any
evidence that an instructional program built around a series (or even a
sequence) of isolated forms is any more supportable now, either theoretically,
empirically, or logically than it was when Krashen and others attacked it
several years ago (p.136: italics in original).”
Long
(1991)
FonF “overtly draws students’ attention to linguistic
elements as they arise incidentally in lessons whose overriding focus is on
meaning or communication” (pp.45-46).
In contrast, FonFs involves traditional language teaching
based on a structural syllabus.
FonFs
|
FonM
|
FonF
|
No needs analysis
|
Usually no needs analysis
|
Needs analysis of target tasks.
|
No realistic models of language
|
Cannot enable older learners to achieve high level of L2
proficiency.
|
Attracts attention to forms that otherwise learners might
not notice.
|
Ignores the fact that learning is a slow and gradual
process.
|
Learners will not acquire non-salient linguistic features.
|
Allows for the slow and gradual process involved in L2
acquisition.
|
Takes no account of constraints on the learnability of
grammar.
|
Learners needs negative evidence to acquire some
grammatical features
|
Respects the learner’s internal syllabus.
|
Tends to result in boring lessons.
|
FonM is inefficient because it results in only slow
progress
|
Is under learner control – occurs in response to
communication problems.
|
Results in more false beginners than finishers.
|
Can result in confidence and fluency in the use of the L2
but limited accuracy.
|
Assists form-function mapping and so promotes both fluency
and accuracy.
|
Summarizing
Long’s early account of “focus on form’
Focus on form:
Ø
Arises in interaction
Ø
Is reactive
Ø
Is incidental
Ø
Is brief
Ø
Is typically implicit
Ø
Induces ‘noticing’
Ø
Induces form-function mapping.
Ø
Constitutes an ‘approach’ to teaching
Subsequent
development (Long, 2015)
“Focus on form involves reactive use of a wide variety of
pedagogic procedures to draw learners’ attention to linguistic problems in
context, as they arise during communication in TBLT, typically as students work
on problem-solving tasks, thereby increasing the likelihood that attention to
code features will be synchronized with the learner’s internal syllabus,
developmental stage and processing ability (p.317).”
Contrasting
Long’s early and late views of ‘focus on form’
Characteristic
|
Early
|
Late
|
Theoretical foundation – attention to form while
communicating
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Approach or procedures?
|
Both
|
Both
|
Reactive and brief?
|
Yes
|
Yes
|
Interactive
|
Yes
|
Yes
but also non-interactive
|
Incidental/intentional learning?
|
Incidental
|
Both
incidental and intentional
|
Implicit/explicit?
|
Implicit
|
Both
implicit and explicit
|
Doughty
and Williams (1998)
·
FonF and FonFs ‘are not polar
opposites’
·
FonF entails a focus on formal elements
of language whereas FonFs is limited to such a focus’ (p.4; italics as in
original).
·
So is PPP FonF or FonFs?
·
PPP = a FonFs approach because it is
based on a structural syllabus and involves the explicit instruction
·
But it can be seen as including a
‘focus on form’ in the final.
Incompatibility
of definitions
But such a view is clearly incompatible with both Long’s
early and later accounts of focus on form.
Long makes a fundamental distinction between a synthetic approach
involving the linear teaching of discrete linguistic features and an analytical
approach where attention to form only emerges out of the efforts to comprehend
and produce meaningful texts in the L2.
Resolving the definitional problem
The problem lies in characterizing FonF and FonFs as
approaches.
Focus on form is best understood not as an approach (i.e. as
FonF) but as involving different kinds of instructional procedures that attract
attention to form during communication.
Focus on forms entails various devices (such as ‘exercises’)
designed to direct learners’ attention to specific forms that are to be studied
and learned as objects.
Planned or unplanned?
In Long’s account ‘focus on form’ is primarily unplanned and
unfocused but the focus on form that occurs in the final P of PPP is
necessarily planned and also highly focused.
However, if we accept that focus on form constitutes a set
of procedures (rather than an approach) we also need to accept that it can be
planned as well as unplanned.
Negotiation of meaning or negotiation of form
Long initially viewed focus on form as arising when communication
problems occurred but subsequently accepts that it can also occur when there is
no communication problem.
So focus on form incorporates both the negotiation of
meaning and the negotiation of form.
Reactive or pre-emptive?
Long also insists that focus on form occurs as a response to
a problem.
But can it also be pre-emptive – as, for example, when a
learner asks a question about a linguistic form during a communicative
activity. Pre-emptive focus on form aims to avoid rather repair a problem. cf.
Ellis, Basturkmen & Loewen (2001)
Interactive focus on form
Interactive focus on form, then, can be defined as the
pre-emptive or responsive procedures that attract attention to form during an
activity that is primarily meaning-focused and that address either
communicative or linguistic problems.
Interactive or non-interactive?
Whereas focus on form was seen as an interactive phenomenon,
it can also clearly also be non-interactive – e.g. text-enhancement procedures.
Obtrusive or non-obtrusive?
Focus on form was initially seen as involving unobtrusive
procedures but is now clearly seen as also including obtrusive procedures (e.g.
metalinguistic explanation).
But should the more obtrusive procedures that direct rather
attract attention to form (e.g. those employed in processing instruction) be
viewed as involving focus-on-forms?
Within or outside communication?
Focus on form was clearly conceived as occurring while
learners were attempting to be communicative.
But as Skehan (1996) noted it can also occur before learners
begin trying to communicate (e.g. through pre-task planning) or after they have
completed a communicative task (e.g. through task repetition).
My definition
· Meaning is primary
· A set of procedures deployed by the
teacher and/or learners to draw attention implicitly or explicitly and often
briefly to problematic linguistic forms
· Can be pre-planned or can arise
incidentally in response to communicative or linguistic problems
· Can be interactive or non-interactive
and involve both production and reception.
· Can be found in both explicit and
implicit approaches to language teaching.
· Can also occur before a communicative
task is performed or while it is being performed.
Selective attention
In focus on form activities attention is selectively focused
on meaning but may also from time to time be voluntarily or involuntarily
focused on specific linguistic forms that occur in the input or that the
learner needs to express a particular meaning.
Role of consciousness
Focus on form caters to incidental/implicit acquisition
(i.e. it may or may not involve consciousness).
Whether noticing of form is needed may depend on the
salience of the linguistic feature (cf. plural –s versus 3rd person –s).
Are different kinds of focus on form needed to facilitate
the acquisition of different linguistic features?
Cognitive comparison
Focus on form can help learners to compare the current state
of their L2 knowledge with the input they are provided with.
‘Cognitive window’ (Doughty & Williams, 1998)
Timing of focus on form
Three possibilities:
1. Prior
to starting to communicate
2. While
communicating
3. Later
after communicative activity is over – but is this focus on form?
Relative effectiveness
If what is important is that learners’ attention to form
takes place while they are primarily engaged in meaning-making (either
receptively or productively), then, focus on form prior or during communication
have merit.
To date, we do not know whether delayed focus on form is
effective.
Working memory
Working memory is limited in capacity and functions as a
site where information can be
1. Temporarily
stored,
2. Rehearsed
to prolong activation
3. Processed
by establishing links with long-term procedural and declarative memories.
Working memory is where ‘intake’ occurs.
Some issues
1. Does
focus on form need to be ‘general’ (as with Skehan) or specific (as with Long)?
2. Does
focus on form facilitate the development of procedural memory (i.e. implicit
knowledge) or
declarative memory (i.e. explicit knowledge) or both?
Revesz’s (2012) study
Revesz investigated the relationship between gains in
grammatical accuracy following recasting.
· Differences in the learners’
phonological working memories were related to accuracy in an oral description
task.
· Differences in their complex working
memory were related to gains in a written test.
How the learners processed the recasts in working memory
affected whether development led to procedural or declarative knowledge.
Doing focus on form
Types of focus on form:
1. Pre-emptive
- Student
initiated
- Teacher
initiated
2. Reactive
- Conversational
- Didactic
Research findings
1. Teachers
are often not aware of the extent to which they engage in focus on form.
2. Both
teachers and learners vary in the extent they engage in focus on form.
3. Both
learners and teachers sometimes make effective use of the learner’s L1 to
address L2
problems.
4. Mainly
focus on form episodes address lexical or grammatical problems rather pragmatic
aspects
of language.
5. The
instructional context affects the frequency with which different focus on form
occurs.
6. Various
factors influence whether learners notice those forms that are focused on – in particular
the
level of explicitness.
7. While
uptake-with-repair cannot be taken as evidence of learning, in some studies it
has been
found to be facilitative of learning.
8. Both
focus on form initiated by learners in learner-learner interaction and by
teachers in whole
-class interaction benefit acquisition.
9. In
interactions involving the teacher pre-emptive focus on form is more effective
than reactive
focus on form if the learner pre-empts.
10. A
stronger effect is evident when the teacher participates in small group work
than in whole-class
interaction.
11. Higher
proficiency learners focus on form more and benefit more from it than lower
proficiency
learners.
Focus of the investigations
1. Text
enhancement
2. Corrective
feedback
3. Pre-task
planning
4. Task
repetition
Key criticisms
1. FonF
is based entirely on theoretical hypotheses that are themselves lacking in
empirical support (Swan, 2005).
2. Focus
on form consists only of quick feedback on learners’ errors while they are
performing a communicative task (Sheen, 2003).
3. Advocates
of FonF present it as the only theoretically-sound way of teaching an L2,
rejecting FonFs entirely (Swan, 2005).
4. There
is no report of any successful long-term implementation of FonF (Sheen, 2005)
5. There
is no evidence to show that FonF resultsin superior L2 learning than FonFs
(Sheen, 1994).
6. FonF
is ill-suited to non-Western cultures of teaching and learning (Littlewood,
2007; 2015)
Two articles
1. Ellis,
R. (2009). Task-based language teaching: Sorting out the misunderstandings.
International Journal of Applied Linguistics 19, 221- 246.
2. Long,
M. H. (2016). In defense of tasks and TBLT: Nonissues and real issues. Annual
Review of Applied Linguistics, 36, 5-33.
Responding to the criticisms
The criticisms demonstrate
· A misunderstanding of focus on form
· Ignorance of the research that has
investigated it.
But the cultural inappropriateness of a focus on form
approach is a more weighty criticism.
Two studies
· Global
vs. local studies
· Two
local studies with very different results:
1.
Sheen (2006)
2.
Shintani (2016)
· Need
for longitudinal studies.
Importance of context
Is FonF appropriate for instructional contexts where:
·
‘Education is conceived more as a
process of knowledge accumulation than as a process of using knowledge for
immediate purposes’? (Littlewood; 2015: 653)
·
Teachers are not familiar with focus on
form?
·
Learners view language as an object
rather than as a tool for communicating?
·
Students are required to take
traditional type tests to progress academically?
Focus on form as ‘procedures’
Focus on form procedures belong naturally task-based
teaching but they can also be found in more traditional approaches providing
there is some opportunity for free production in the L2.
We urgently need studies that compare focus on form
treatment that include and exclude explicit instruction with care taken to
measure the effects on the acquisition of both explicit and implicit knowledge.
Comments
Post a Comment